diff options
author | grr <grr@lo2.org> | 2024-05-02 20:51:35 -0400 |
---|---|---|
committer | grr <grr@lo2.org> | 2024-05-02 20:51:35 -0400 |
commit | 8548929f5fcdc5dde833337247b223ce614a8199 (patch) | |
tree | a75b73b20cb25752c7b09fcd708dc33c598a82a5 /essays/exercise_awareness_states.tex | |
parent | c14e19c8343f3f00944b9d50840501906c4ae2d7 (diff) | |
download | blueprint-8548929f5fcdc5dde833337247b223ce614a8199.tar.gz |
para-science breakout
Diffstat (limited to 'essays/exercise_awareness_states.tex')
-rw-r--r-- | essays/exercise_awareness_states.tex | 293 |
1 files changed, 293 insertions, 0 deletions
diff --git a/essays/exercise_awareness_states.tex b/essays/exercise_awareness_states.tex new file mode 100644 index 0000000..a904da1 --- /dev/null +++ b/essays/exercise_awareness_states.tex @@ -0,0 +1,293 @@ +HENRY FLYNT +Exercise Awareness-States (July 1961) + + +The July 1967 issue of IKON contained Henry Flynt’s “Mock Risk Games.” +This work was a reconstruction, from memory, of Flynt’s 1961 work, “Exer- +cise Awareness-States,” which Flynt had disavowed and discarded in 1962. +In 1981 Flynt obtained a copy of the original 1961 piece in the possession +of Tom Constanten. |n this issue IKON publishes, for the first time, the +original “Exercise Awareness-States.” Preceding the text, we reprint the +introduction from “Mock Risk Games” because of its clarity in explaining the +work. Flynt read “Exercise Awareness-States” during his July 15, 1961 +appearance in the legendary series at George Maciunas’ A/G Gallery, NYC. +This was the only documented public presentation of the work in that period. +The reconstruction “Mock Risk Games” has been printed a number of +times—it was included in Flynt’s book, Blueprint For a Higher Civilization +(Milan, 1975). + + +INTRODUCTION (from “Mock Risk Games”—1967 Version) + + +Suppose you stand in front of a swinging door with a nail sticking out of it +pointing at your face; and suppose you are prepared to jump back if the door +suddenly opens in your face. You are deliberately taking a risk on the +assumption that you can protect yourself. Let us call such a situation a “risk +game.” Then a “mock risk game” is a risk game such that the misfortune +which you risk is contrary to the course of nature, a freak misfortune; and +thus your preparation to evade it is correspondingly superficial. + +lf the direction of gravity reverses and you fall on the ceiling, that is a freak +misfortune. If you don’t want to risk this misfortune, then you will anchor +yourself to the floor in some way. But if you stand free so that you can fall, +and yet try to prepare so that if you do fall, you will fall in such a way that +you won't be hurt, then that is a mock risk game. If technicians could actually +effect or simulate gravity reversal in the room, then the risk game would be +a real one. But | am not concerned with real risk games. | am interested in +dealing with gravity reversal in an everyday environment, where everything +tells you it can’t possibly happen. Your “preparation” for the fall is thus +superficial, because you still have the involuntary conviction that it can’t +possibly happen. + +Mock risk games constitute a new area of human behavior, because they +aren't something people have done before you don’t know what they will be +like until you try them, and it took a very special effort to devise them. They +have a tremendous advantage over other activities of comparable sig- +nificance, because they can be produced in the privacy of your own room +without special equipment. Let us explore this new psychological effect; and +let us not ask what use it has until we are more familiar with it. + +Instructions for a variety of mock risk games follow. (I have played each +game many times in developing it, to ensure that the experience of playing +it will be compelling.) For each game, there is a physical action to be + + +71 + + +72 + + +performed in a physical setting. Then there is a list of freak misfortunes +which you risk by performing the action, and which you must be prepared +to evade. The point is not to hallucinate the misfortunes, or even to fear +them, but rather to be prepared to evade them. First you work with each +misfortune separately. For example, you walk across a room, prepared to +react self-protectingly if you are suddenly upside down, resting on the top +of your head on the floor. In preparing for this risk, you should clear the path +of objects that might hurt you if you fell on them, you should wear clothes +suitable for falling, and you should try standing on your head, taking your +hands off the floor and falling, to get a feeling for how to fall without getting +hurt. After you have mastered the preparation for each misfortune separate- +ly, you perform the action prepared to evade the first misfortune and the +second (but not both at once). You must prepare to determine instantly +which of the two misfortunes befalls you, and to react appropriately. After +you have mastered pairs of misfortunes, you go on to triples of misfortunes, +and so forth. + + +EXERCISE AWARENESS-STATES (July, 1961) + + +| am concerned here to introduce an activity which | will call, for want of +a better term, “exercise,” and the states of awareness one has in exercise, +“exercise awareness-states.” Incidentally, this activity is based on wrong, +although common, philosophical assumptions, but | hope the reader will play +along with them for the sake of the activity; philosophical rightness is not +the main concern here. Exercise should be thought of first as training to help +prepare one for dangerous situations of a very special kind (which the +reader is admittedly not likely to encounter). (Incidentally, ’danger’ here +should not be an emotive word; my concern is with the theory of defense, +not with giving the reader vicarious experience.) Suppose that the adults in +a society occasionally have to be in situations, such as walking across a +bare metal floor in a certain “building,” during which dangers, very unusual +and unpredictable, may arise. Suppose that they know nothing of the +provenance of the dangers, just that they may be there, so that they can’t +prevent them (or predict what they will be); the persons are somewhat like +animals trying to defend themselves against a variety of modern (human) +weapons. They cannot adequately prepare for the dangers by practicing +responses to specific dangers so that they become habitual, because of the +extreme unpredictability of the actual dangers. However, the dangers are +such that when one arises a person can figure out what he needs to do to +defend himself fast enough and carry it out. + +Finally, suppose that although it is desired to train persons [to be prepared] +to defend themselves in the situations, there isn’t the technology to simulate +dangers, so that they can’t be given a chance to actually figure out and carry +out defenses against simulated dangers. Then it would seem that the best +preparation in the situations (until a danger appeared) would be the state +of mind—"unpredictably-dangerous-situation awareness state"—of lack of +preconceptions as to what one might encounter, emotionlessness (except +for the small amount of fear and confidence needed to make one maximally +alert), very very heightened awareness of all sensory data, and readiness +to figure out (quickly) whether they indicated a danger and [to figure out] a + + +defense against it. After all, it might be best to stay away, or at least get +away, from the preparation resulting from practice with simulated dangers, +just because the actual ones are so unpredictable. Training for the situations +would then be to help persons achieve this best dangerous-situation aware- +ness- state when in the situations. Then (one should first think) the purpose +of “exercise,” or the “exercises,” is to help persons to achieve the best +dangerous-situation awareness-state in the situations by teaching them to +achieve “u/timate exercise awareness-states,” which are as similar as pos- +sible to the best dangerous-situation states within the limitations | have +given. + +Exercise may secondly be thought of as something to be done for its own +sake, so that ultimate exercise awareness-states are achieved for their own +sake, in particular, as an unusual way of “appreciating” the sensory date +while in them. This is the way | suppose the reader will regard exercise. Thus +exercise, rather than unpredictably dangerous situations, is the principal +subject of this paper. However, it should not be lost sight of that exercise +could be useful in the first way; and the development of exercises should be +controlled by concern with whether they are useful in the first way. + +| will now give some explanations and general instructions for exercise. +An “exercise” is what the general instructions, and a specification of a(n +exercise) “situation” one is to place oneself in and of several “given dan- +gers” to anticipate in the situation, refers to; an “exercise awareness-state” +is any state of mind throughout an exercise. In first doing an exercise, one +anticipates given dangers; the point of having specific dangers to anticipate +at first is to keep one from anticipating nothing, being indifferent in the +situation and thus not achieving an interesting awareness-state. In a good +exercise, the dangers should be interesting to anticipate, one should find it +easy to anticipate them strongly, and it should be clear what is dangerous +in them and how they can defended against. It is only when one can +anticipate the given dangers strongly that one does the exercise, places +oneself in the situation, without thinking of specific dangers, trying to strong- +ly anticipate unpredictable danger; when one can do this one will be achiev- +ing “ultimate exercise awareness-states.” + +The general instructions for the exercises follow. First place oneself in the +situation, anticipate one of the given dangers as strongly as possible (short +of getting oneself in a state of fright), be very very aware of all sensory data, +and be ready to figure out (quickly) whether they indicate the danger and to +start defending against it. Try to achieve the greatest anticipation of and +readiness for the one danger. The result is an “initial exercise awareness- +state.” Finally one can do the exercise forgetting the given dangers; place +oneself in the situation, try to anticipate [unpredictable] danger strongly +(short of getting oneself frightened), without preconceptions as to what form +it will take, be very very aware of all sense data, and be ready to figure out +(quickly) whether they indicate a danger, and a defense against it. This is +an “ultimate exercise awareness-state.” A final point. So that one will not be +distracted from the exercise, there must be a minimum of familiar events +extraneous to it during it, such as the sight of a door opening, talking, +cooking smells. For this reason, unless otherwise stated exercise should be +taken in environments as inanimate, quiet, odorless, etc. as possible. One +will fail to achieve interesting exercise awareness-states if one cannot play + + +73 + + +74 + + +along and (for the sake of the exercise) strongly anticipate danger; [because +one doesn’t expect it,] but rather remains relaxed, indifferent, or worse is +sleepy, physiologically depressed (indifferent, depressed exercise states). +It should be clear that one has to really try the exercises, not just read about +them, in order to appreciate them. + + +EXERCISE 1 + +The situation: You walk across the floor of a medium-sized brightly lighted +square room, from the middle of one side to the middle of the other, in a +straight line. There should be no other animal [fauna, animate creature] in +the room and the path of walking should be clear [of obstructions]; ideally +the room should be bare. + +The given dangers to anticipate: +(1) Heavy invisible objects falling around you, making a whirring noise as +they do. +(2) Immovability of whichever foot presses most strongly on the floor, and +a steel cylinder two feet in diameter with sharp edges’ falling down, around +you (hopefully). +(3) Instantaneous inversion of yourself so that you rest on whatever part of +your surface was uppermost in walking, and doubling of the gravitational +force on you. +(4) Sudden dizziness, change of equilibrium to that of one who has been +turning around for a long time, and the floor’s vanishing except for a narrow +strip, where you have walked, shortening from the front. +(5) Change of field of vision to behind your head, instead of in front, +something’s coming to hit you from the side in an erratic path, and loud +noises on the side of you opposite it. +(6) What you see’s suddenly becoming two-dimensional instead of three so +that you bump into it, while the room fills from behind with a mildly toxic gas; +and going forward’s requiring that you guess the unpredictable action, +symbolic of getting past the barrier, which will enable you to get forward. + + +EXERCISE 2 + +You stand, in a dark room, facing a wall and pulling medium hard with both +hands on a horizontal bar running along the wall and attached to it, for five +minutes. Have an alarm clock to let you know when the time is up. There +should be no other animal in the room; ideally the room should be bare. You +must not let up on the pulling; the assumption is that if you do your eyes +and ears will be assaulted with a blinding light and a deafening sound, +except in the case of certain dangers. + +The dangers: +(1) Loss of your kinesthetic sense. (body-movement or muscle sense) +(2) Suspension of the “normal” “cause and effect” relationship between +pulling and keeping the light and sound from appearing, so that you just +have to guess what to do to keep them from appearing and it changes with +time, with the restriction that it will be closely related to pulling on the bar, +e.g. letting go of the bar. +(3) Suspension of the “normal” “cause and effect” relationship between what +you will and what your body does, so that you just have to guess what to +will to keep your arms (and hands) pulling on the bar and it changes with + + +time, with the restriction that it will be closely related to willing to pull on +the bar, e.g. willing to let go of the bar. + +(4) Having the tactile, cutaneous sensation of being under water, so that +you will “drown”’—"cutaneously"—unless you cutaneously swim to the top; +your sight and hearing being lost except for sensitivity to the light and sound +if you stop pulling. + + +EXERCISE 3 + +The situation: You lie on your back, barefoot, on a bunk, your arms more +or less at your sides, with a pillow on your face so that you can breathe but +not easily, for five minutes. Do not change your position; the assumption is +that you can’t except in the case of certain dangers. Have an alarm clock +to let you know when the time is up. The room should be dark and there +should be no other animal in it. Ideally you should be lying, in the middle +and along the longitudinal axis of a not uncomfortably hard rectangular +surface a yard above the floor and having an area almost that of the room, +in a very long room, which should otherwise be bare. + +The dangers: +(1) The gravitational force’s becoming zero and the room’s getting un- +bearably hot towards the ceiling. +(2) Having to press the pillow against your face with your arms and hands, +except for one angle of your face wherein you can roll your face from under +the pillow, your head and neck becoming movable. +(3) The surface you are lying on’s and the pillow’s turning into a two-part +living organism, of which the lower part is so delicate that unless you +distribute your bodily pressure on it as evenly as possible, it will be injured +and the upper part will pull you off of it by the skin of your face in +self-protection, the organism being sufficiently telepathic that you can +sense when it is hurting. +(4) Division of your body (and clothing) just below the ribs. The two halves +separate by 114 feet and a metal wall one inch thick appears between them. +Matter and so forth are transmitted between halves and they remain in the +usual position relative to each other so that it is rather as if you simply grew +in the middle by 112 feet. Your consciousness suddenly seems to be located +in the pillow, where the pillow is, rather than in your head; nothing that +happens to the pillow materially affects your consciousness. Two kinds of +metal blocks come crashing against the wall from far in front of and behind +it, starting slowly and speeding up as they get near the wall, and then draw +back to where they came from. Blocks of the first kind come from the front +(the side the upper part of your body and the pillow are on) only; they are +“vertical,” tall and narrow so that they can be avoided by moving from side +to side. Blocks of the second kind come in pairs, one in front, one behind. +They are “horizontal,” two feet high (thick), and very wide (long). The ones +in front hit low and the ones in back high, so they can be avoided by standing +up (necessarily in a stooped position). Each time the pair hits higher and +higher. There are long indentations in the back side of the wall in which one +can get footholds to climb the wall. If one gets to the top of the wall, gets +both halves of one’s body above the wall, they will rejoin. + + +75 + + + |